Real images are always located behind the mirror

Collection of focus points made by converging light rays

Real images are always located behind the mirror

Top: The formation of a real image using a convex lens. Bottom: The formation of a real image using a concave mirror. In both diagrams, f  is the focal point, O  is the object, and I  is the image. Solid blue lines indicate light rays. It can be seen that the image is formed by actual light rays and thus can form a visible image on a screen placed at the position of the image.

Real images are always located behind the mirror

An inverted real image of distant house, formed by a convex lens, is viewed directly without being projected onto a screen.

Real images are always located behind the mirror

Producing a real image. Each region of the detector or retina indicates the light produced by a corresponding region of the object.

In optics, an image is defined as the collection of focus points of light rays coming from an object. A real image is the collection of focus points actually made by converging/diverging rays, while a virtual image is the collection of focus points made by extensions of diverging or converging rays. In other words, it is an image which is located in the plane of convergence for the light rays that originate from a given object. Examples of real images include the image produced on a detector in the rear of a camera, and the image produced on an eyeball retina (the camera and eye focus light through an internal convex lens).

In ray diagrams (such as the images on the right), real rays of light are always represented by full, solid lines; perceived or extrapolated rays of light are represented by dashed lines. A real image occurs where rays converge, whereas a virtual image occurs where rays only appear to diverge.

Real images can be produced by concave mirrors and converging lenses, only if the object is placed further away from the mirror/lens than the focal point, and this real image is inverted. As the object approaches the focal point the image approaches infinity, and when the object passes the focal point the image becomes virtual and is not inverted (upright image). The distance is not the same as from the object to the lenses.

Real images may also be inspected by a second lens or lens system. This is the mechanism used by telescopes, binoculars and light microscopes. The objective lens gathers the light from the object and projects a real image within the structure of the optical instrument. A second lens or system of lenses, the eyepiece, then projects a second real image onto the retina of the eye.

See also

  • Focal plane
  • Image plane
  • Lens
  • Virtual image

Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Real_image&oldid=1082332129"

Real images are always located behind the mirror

Real images are always located behind the mirror
Real images are always located behind the mirror

Get the answer to your homework problem.

Try Numerade free for 7 days

Is there a simple way to determine or prove this? Real images are always inverted, and unlike virtual images can be projected onto a screen (I'm not even sure what this means to be honest). If I look at the back end of a spoon (convex mirror), the image is always upright and therefore virtual, in back of the mirror. Makes sense, since I'm in front of the mirror. Then I turn to the concave side of the spoon and keep it less than one focal length away from me, still the image is virtual and upright, and supposedly still in back of the mirror. But after moving the mirror more than a focal length away from me the image is now inverted, and therefore real. Does this mean it's in front of the mirror? I can't tell the difference. Please clarify and correct any misstatements/misassumptions.

Answers and Replies

sophiecentaur

Real images are always inverted, and unlike virtual images can be projected onto a screen

That is not always practical when a curved mirror is concerned. Light from an object would very often have to pass through the screen in order for it to reflected onto the screen. Spoons are a great example of where it really doesn't work very well. They are handy though and you can play at the dinner table - people may well think you are loopy though. Take some time and you will make it work. You can resolve all this if you use the Parallax Method for finding the position of an image. This YouTube movie shows how the virtual image in the mirror appears to be at the same depth as the (finder) red pole that is actually placed behind the mirror. There is only one position for the finder where the image coincides with it from all viewing angles so that's where the image is. This demo can be scaled down and a small rectangular mirror can be stood on a soft wood board and pins can be used to get the effect. In school, we did this easily and could locate the position of the image by moving our heads from side to side. The same method works for concave and convex mirrors and lenses. If you hold a spoon up with your thumb near the centre of the bowl, you can put it at a place where there is no parallax with the image of a window or lamp. Better still. a concave 'shaving mirror' has less curvature and you can easily put your finger in the no-parallax position with the image of a distant object. If you put a thin tissue or tracing paper, covering about half the mirror, at the right spot, you can also focus the image of a window or lamp on the tissue and you can see it through the tissue.

Nowadays, kids are often given laser pointers for this sort of experiment but they actually fail to beat the Pin method when you need understanding.

Likes Fizzizist

Is there a simple way to determine or prove this? Real images are always inverted, and unlike virtual images can be projected onto a screen (I'm not even sure what this means to be honest). If I look at the back end of a spoon (convex mirror), the image is always upright and therefore virtual, in back of the mirror. Makes sense, since I'm in front of the mirror. Then I turn to the concave side of the spoon and keep it less than one focal length away from me, still the image is virtual and upright, and supposedly still in back of the mirror. But after moving the mirror more than a focal length away from me the image is now inverted, and therefore real. Does this mean it's in front of the mirror? I can't tell the difference. Please clarify and correct any misstatements/misassumptions.

There is nothing to correct, you got it basically right. The only thing you need to understand is why you cannot tell the difference between the real and the virtual image formed by the concave mirror. Think about how your eye perceives images. Suppose you look at the reflection of an upright arrow in a concave mirror. Regardless of where the arrow is all rays are reflected from the mirror. Slightly divergent rays coming from the tip of the arrow are bent by the lens of your eye and focused on your retina. Your brain interprets the stimulus from the light as a spot and connects all nearby spots o the interpretation of an arrow as there is a one-to-one correspondence of light leaving a point on the arrow and a point where that light gets mapped on your retina. Your brain has learned to extend nearly parallel but divergent rays to their point of intersection in front of you and that's where you "see" the image to be. In the case of a virtual image, the rays from the arrow bounce off the mirror and reach your eye without ever converging before they are focused on your retina. In the case of a real image, the rays from the arrow bounce off the mirror, converge in front of the mirror once to form the real image, then they spread out again and finally reach your eye where they converge once again on your retina. That's why you cannot tell the difference.

Likes Fizzizist

Merlin3189

... why you cannot tell the difference between the real and the virtual image formed by the concave mirror. .... Slightly divergent rays coming from the tip of the arrow are bent by the lens of your eye and focused on your retina.

... Your brain has learned to extend nearly parallel but divergent rays to their point of intersection in front of you and that's where you "see" the image to be. .

How can your brain know what direction individual rays are coming from, if they are all focused to a single spot? All it can know is that spot is getting light from somewhere. If the rods and cones could (they can't) in some way tell the directions of at least some rays, they would see converging rays, not diverging ones. It does not matter how far away the source, nor how divergent the rays from it *, the lens of the eye bends them so that they converge at the same rate, to form a single point on the retina. The only thing the brain could learn from the angle of convergence, is the distance between the retina and the lens. I think the only information the eye can get about the divergence of the rays coming from the object, is the amount it has to stretch the lens to get the rays to focus to a point. This is a recognised cue to depth perception, but is limited in its range and IMO not a major cue. Spectacle wearers may experience a momentary sensation of movement when they put on or remove their specs, but I don't think spectacles generally impair ones depth perception, even though they significantly change the divergence of rays and the degree of accommodation of the lens. Even wearing varifocal lenses does not seem to interfere with depth perception. What I suspect the brain has learned, is to adjust the stretching of the lens to get the greatest contrast in the centre of the image, much as digital cameras do. It's hard to get a single point of light without any other cues to distance, to test one's distance perception using just accommodation of the lens. The nearest I can think of at the moment, is looking at a star through a telescope. I don't think I'm aware of how far away the image is, but perhaps that's because I've never thought about it. ** Other than that I'd agree that there is no property of the eye and the formation of the retinal image that distinguishes between real and virtual images. To the eye (as to another lens) an image is as real as an object. You can tell the position of an image in the same way as you can tell the position of an object and if you understand a bit about optics, your brain deduces whether it is real or virtual. ============================== * - All assuming the object is within the range that can be focused: otherwise they don't converge to a single spot anyway.

** - It's not because I have the telescope in normal adjustment - I'm myopic, so I need an image about 50 cm away.

Likes Fizzizist

How can your brain know what direction individual rays are coming from, if they are all focused to a single spot?

Individual rays stimulate individual spots on the retina that can be assigned separate spherical angles θ and φ, if you wish. When I look up to the night sky, I can see one distant star by looking right at it and one with the corner of my right eye. Each star forms an image on my retina at different θ and φ. My conclusion is that one star is straight ahead and the other to my right.

Likes Fizzizist

Merlin3189

Sorry, I thought you were talking about all the rays from a single point (or single star) diverging to your pupil and then being converged to a single point on your retina. I shall have to reread your post.

Individual rays stimulate individual spots on the retina ....

I was assuming, for objects such as your arrow, each point on the object sent out millions of rays which were then focused (mapped?) to a single point on the retina. I suppose, if it were dim enough to emit a single photon at a time, or if your pupil were small enough (& ignoring diffraction), then maybe we could say there was only a single ray from each point on the object to each point on the image. I can understand you can tell two stars apart, as even if they each emit countless rays, those rays may end up at two distinct spots on the retina. But it's not worth discussing further, as it relates to visual perception rather than real and virtual images that op asked about.

As far as that goes, I don't think where the image is nor what it looks like is important. Simply whether the light passes through the image, or would do so in the absence of other obstructions (including such as a second mirror or lens.) An objective criterion rather than a subjective one.

Likes Fizzizist

Is there a simple way to determine or prove this? Real images are always inverted, and unlike virtual images can be projected onto a screen (I'm not even sure what this means to be honest). If I look at the back end of a spoon (convex mirror), the image is always upright and therefore virtual, in back of the mirror. Makes sense, since I'm in front of the mirror. Then I turn to the concave side of the spoon and keep it less than one focal length away from me, still the image is virtual and upright, and supposedly still in back of the mirror. But after moving the mirror more than a focal length away from me the image is now inverted, and therefore real. Does this mean it's in front of the mirror? I can't tell the difference. Please clarify and correct any misstatements/misassumptions.

Concave mirror gives you a real image at the focal point (for object at infinity). When you are looking at the concave side of the spoon, you are way beyond the focal point. It is why you saw an image with your eye. Inside the focal point, you'll never be able to see a focused image with your eye. Draw a proper ray diagram to convince yourself of this, or do a Google search.

Zz.

Likes Fizzizist

I suppose, if it were dim enough to emit a single photon at a time, or if your pupil were small enough (& ignoring diffraction), then maybe we could say there was only a single ray from each point on the object to each point on the image.

I think it is dangerous to put too much faith in the ray model in that limit. I prefer to model it as many rays distributed over an area element dA of the pupil much like electric field lines are distributed over an area when one considers electric flux. To me a single ray is as meaningful as a single electric field line.

But it's not worth discussing further, as it relates to visual perception rather than real and virtual images that op asked about.

I agree.

Likes Fizzizist

Great info everyone, thanks. So the eye can't tell the difference. A movie is a real image, while a mirage is virtual. A plane mirror is always upright and therefore virtual: the source of light is in front of the mirror but it appears in back. Does the curvature reflect the light more than once in the concave mirror, changing the source so it's in back? Somewhat like the reason letters appear backwards in a mirror?

Mister T

Real images are always inverted, and unlike virtual images can be projected onto a screen (I'm not even sure what this means to be honest).

Try to form an image on a screen with a convex mirror, you never can. Try with a concave mirror and you sometimes can.

Make the ray-tracing diagrams. When the rays "really" converge to form an image, that's a real image. When they don't, it isn't.

Likes Fizzizist

Mister T

If I look at the back end of a spoon (convex mirror), the image is always upright and therefore virtual,[...]

The use of "therefore" doesn't seem right to me. The image is always upright and the image is always virtual, but I don't see how either one of those is the cause of the other.

[...] in back of the mirror. Makes sense, since I'm in front of the mirror.

I don't follow. The image is behind the mirror and you are in front of the mirror. But I don't understand what connection between the two leads you to conclude that that makes sense. It does make sense, but I wonder why it makes sense to you. The reason it makes sense to me is that when I draw the ray diagram I see that the rays appear to have originated from an image, and the place where that happens is behind the mirror.

Then I turn to the concave side of the spoon and keep it less than one focal length away from me, still the image is virtual and upright, and supposedly still in back of the mirror.

No supposition involved. Draw the ray diagram and see for yourself that the rays appear to come from an image, that that image is indeed behind the mirror and that that image is upright.

But after moving the mirror more than a focal length away from me the image is now inverted, and therefore real.


Being inverted is not the cause of it being real. What makes it real is that the rays "really" do converge to form an image. And when you draw the ray diagram you do indeed see that the image is inverted.

Likes Fizzizist

Try with a concave mirror and you sometimes can.

This is easier with a large-diameter mirror and a small-diameter screen. When I was teaching, as part of my lecture-demonstration equipment, I had a concave mirror about 40-50cm in diameter. I could hold up a card about 5x5cm in size, at the image location, and get a nice image on it.

Last edited: May 10, 2018

Likes Fizzizist

Try with a concave mirror and you sometimes can.

Is there any way to hold the spoon near a wall so it does? Or is some other method needed?

The image is always upright and the image is always virtual, but I don't see how either one of those is the cause of the other.

I don't either, it's just something to accept without proof and hope everything reconciles later on. A common but suboptimal learning technique :O

How do I draw the ray diagram? Measure the object and image distances from the mirror, and also that of the focal length? Is it even possible to measure the image distance without a sensor/computer?

Mister T

How do I draw the ray diagram?

I just typed "drawing a ray diagram" into the google search engine. The first hit is a lesson in how to do it.

The task itself is not particularly hard, although it does take a lot of practice, but it is hard to explain it in a post. The time spent learning this is time well spent because it helps you understand what the equations are telling you.

sophiecentaur

How do I draw the ray diagram?

Are you doing a course on optics or is this a 'one-off' enquiry? There is far too much to learn about optics for you to progress by a Q and A method, using questions that happen to strike you as important.
As @Mister T says, you can find loads of sources that will show you what to do and give you examples to solve - which will show you how well you have understood what they tell you.

Googling is nice, but I like direct answers to questions (which you all have been kindly providing!) This isn't homework, just me trying to understand the concepts better.